James Mckeen Cattell Pdf

On

American psychologist James McKeen Cattell. (1860–1944) studied with Galton and then, in 1890, proclaimed the modern testing agenda in his classic. The Birth of Psychometrics in Cambridge, 1886 - 1889. It is a little-known fact that psychometrics as a science began in Cambridge between 1886 and 1889. The first laboratory dedicated to the subject was set up within the Cavendish Physics Laboratory at the University of Cambridge by James McKeen Cattell in 1887.

The purpose of this article is to describe the links between late nineteenth-century psychological research and the early musical aptitude research of Carl Emil Seashore (1866-1949). The primary link was the music-related research of the leader of the mental testing movement during the 1890s, Columbia University psychologist James McKeen Cattell (1860-1944). German psychologist Wilhelm Wundt instructed Cattell in the German scientific tradition, and English researcher Francis Galton encouraged Cattell's research on individual differences and introduced him to statistical methods. During the 1890s, Cattell conducted a longitudinal study, the hypothesis for which was that tests of sensory discrimination ability, including musical discrimination, would correlate with undergraduates' academic grades. After his study failed to produce the expected results, the mental testing movement followed Alfred Binet and Victor Henri of France, and Cattell turned to other activities. However, in the meantime, Cattell influenced many other important psychologists, including Edward W. Scripture, Carl Seashore's doctoral mentor at Yale University, and eventually Seashore himself. Despite the mental testing movement's shift to Binet and Henri's cognitive-type testing, Seashore continued his conservative, sensory approach to the testing of musical aptitude.

Binet, A. , and V. Henri. 'La psychologie individuelle.' L'année psychologique 2 (1895): 411-65. Excerpts translated by Mollie D. Boring. In A Source Book in the History of Psychology , ed. Richard J. Herrnstein and Edwin G. Boring, 428-33. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1965.
Google Scholar Crossref
Boas, Frank. 'Anthropological Investigations in Schools.' Pedagogical Seminary 1 (June 1891): 225-28.
Google Scholar Crossref
Boring, Edwin G. A History of Experimental Psychology. 2d ed. New York: Appleton Century-Crofts, 1950.
Google Scholar
Cattell, J. McKeen. 'Early Psychological Laboratories.' Science 67 (May 1928): 543-48.
Google Scholar Crossref Medline
—. 'The First Year of the Psychological Corporation.' Unpublished report to the Board of Directors, 1 December 1922. James McKeen Cattell Collection, Container #178, 'Subject File, 1890-1936. Manuscript Division, Library of Congress, Washington, DC.
Google Scholar
—. James McKeen Cattell , 1860-1944: Man of Science. Vol. I (Lancaster, PA: The Science Press , 1947).
Google Scholar
—. Cambridge [England], to 'Mama and Papa' [William and Elizabeth Cattell], Philadelphia, 23 November 1893, 25 July 1894 , 5 December 1894. Letters in the hand of James McKeen Cattell. James McKeen Cattell Collection, Container #55, ' Family Correspondence,' December 1888-March 1903 . Manuscript Division, Library of Congress , Washington, DC. (Two of these letters are misfiled in the Collection.)
Google Scholar
—. Leipzig [Germany], to Francis Galton, Cambridge [England], [n.d. given]. Quoted in Francis Galton. 'On Recent Designs for Anthropometric Instruments.' The Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 16 (1887): 2-9.
Google Scholar Crossref
—. 'In Memory of Wilhelm Wundt.' Psychological Review 28 (May 1921): 155-59.
Google Scholar
—. 'Mental Tests and Measurements.' Mind 15 (January 1890): 373-80.
Google Scholar
—. 'The Psychological Laboratory at Leipsic [sic].' Mind 13 (January 1888): 37-51.
Google Scholar
—. 'Psychology at the University of Pennsylvania.' American Journal of Psychology 3 (April 1890): 281-83.
Google Scholar
—. 'Ueber die Trägheit der Netzhaut und des Sehcentrums.' Philosophische Studien 3 (1885): 94-127. Reprinted as ' The Inertia of the Eye and Brain.' In (no translator) James McKeen Cattell. James McKeen Cattell, 1860-1944: Man of Science. Vol. I, 26-40. Lancaster, PA: The Science Press, 1947.
Google Scholar
—. 'Ueber die Zeit der Erkennung und Benennung von Schriftzeichen, Bildem und Farben' Philosophische Studien 2 (1885): 635-50. Excerpts translated by R. S. Woodworth in 'On the Time Required for Recognizing and Naming Letters and Words, Pictures and Colors' In James McKeen Cattell. James McKeen Cattell, 1860-1944: Man of Science. Vol. I, 13-25. Lancaster, PA: The Science Press, 1947.
Google Scholar
Cattell, J. McKeen , and Livingston Farrand . 'Physical and Mental Measurements of the Students of Columbia University. Psychological Review 3 (1896): 618-48.
Google Scholar Crossref
Darwin, Charles. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex, 2d ed. New York: American Publishers Corporation, 1874.
Google Scholar Crossref
Deary, Ian J. 'Intelligence and Auditory Discrimination: Separating Processing Speed and Fidelity of Stimulus Representation.' Intelligence 18 (March 1994): 189-213.
Google Scholar Crossref ISI
DuBois, Philip H. A History of Psychological Testing. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1970.
Google Scholar
Esquirol, Jean-Etienne Dominique. Des maladies mentales conside rées sous les rapports médical, hygienique, et médico-légal. Vols. I, II and Atlas. Paris: J. B. Bailliére, 1838.
Google Scholar
Galton, Francis. 'On Recent Designs for Anthropometric Instruments.' The Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 16 (1887): 2-9.
Google Scholar Crossref
Gilbert, J.A. 'Experiments on the Musical Sensitiveness of School Children.' In Studies from the Yale Psychological Laboratory. Vol. I, ed. E. W. Scripture , 80-87. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1893.
Google Scholar
—. 'Researches on Mental and Physical Development of School-Children.' Studies from the Yale Psychological Laboratory. Vol. II, ed. E. W. Scripture , 40-100. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1894.
Google Scholar
—. 'Researches upon School Children and College Students.' University of Iowa Studies in Psychology. Vol. I, ed. G. T. W. Patrick and J. Allen Gilbert , 1-39. Iowa City: State University of Iowa, 1897.
Google Scholar
Goodenough, Florence L. Mental Testing: Its History, Principles, and Applications . New York: Rinehart and Company , 1949.
Google Scholar
Gordon, Edwin. Musical Aptitude Profile. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1965.
Google Scholar
Herrnstein, Richard J. I.Q. in the Meritocracy. Boston: Little, Brown, 1973.
Google Scholar
Herrnstein, Richard J. , and Edwin G. Boring , eds. A Source Book in the History of Psychology . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1965.
Google Scholar
Humphreys, Jere T. 'Applications of Science: The Age of Standardization and Efficiency in Music Education.' The Bulletin of Historical Research in Music Education 9 (January 1988): 1-21.
Google Scholar
—. 'The Child-Study Movement and Public School Music Education.' Journal of Research in Music Education 33 (Summer 1985): 79-86.
Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI
—. 'Precursors of Musical Aptitude Testing: From the Greeks through the Work of Francis Galton.' Journal of Research in Music Education 41 (Winter 1993): 315-27.
Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI
Humphreys, Jere T. , William V. May , and David J. Nelson . 'Research on Music Ensembles.' In Handbook of Research on Music Teaching and Learning, ed. Richard Colwell , 651-68. New York: Schirmer Books, 1992.
Google Scholar
Jastrow, Joseph. 'Joseph Jastrow.' In A History of Psychology in Autobiography . Vol. I, ed. Carl Murchison , 135-62. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press, 1930; reprint, New York : Russell & Russell, 1961.
Google Scholar Crossref
—. 'Some Anthropometric and Psychologic [sic] Tests on College Students .' American Journal of Psychology 4 (April 1892): 420-28.
Google Scholar
Linden, Katherine W. , and James D. Linden . Modern Mental Measurement: A Historical Perspective . Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company , 1968.
Google Scholar
Münsterberg, Hugo. 'Zur individual Psychologie,' Centralblatt f. Nervenheilkunde und Psychiatrie 14 (1891): 196-98.
Google Scholar
Mursell, James L. The Psychology of School Music Teaching. New York: Silver, Burdett and Company, 1931.
Google Scholar
Pearson, Karl. The Life, Letters and Labours of Francis Galton. Vol. IIIA. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1930.
Google Scholar
Scripture, [Edward Wheeler], New Haven, CT, to [James McKeen] Cattell, New York, NY, 7 August 1901. Letter in the hand of Edward Wheeler Scripture. James McKeen Cattell Collection, Container #38, 'General Correspondence,' 1884-1944. Manuscript Division, Library of Congress, Washington, DC.
Google Scholar
—. 'The Method of Regular Variation,' in 'Psychological Notes' American Journal of Psychology 4 (August 1892): 577-84.
Google Scholar Crossref
__. The New Psychology. New York: Scribner's , 1897.
Google Scholar
Seashore, Carl Emil. 'Carl Emil Seashore.' In A History of Psychology in Autobiography. Vol. I, ed. Carl Murchison , 225-97. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press, 1930; reprint, New York : Russell & Russell, 1961.
Google Scholar
—. ' Hearing-ability and Discriminative Sensibility for Pitch.' University of Iowa Studies in Psychology. Vol. II, ed. G. T. W. Patrick , 163-78. Iowa City: State University of Iowa, 1899.
Google Scholar
—. 'Measurements of Illusions and Hallucinations in Normal Life.' Ph.D. diss., Yale University, 1895; published in Studies from the Yale Psychological Laboratory. Vol. III, ed. E. W. Scripture , 1-67. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1895.
Google Scholar
Seashore, Carl E. Seashore Measures of Musical Talent. New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1919.
Google Scholar Crossref
Sokal, Michael M. 'Biographical Approach: The Psychological Career of Edward Wheeler Scripture.' In Historiography of Modern Psychology: Aims, Resources, Approaches, ed. Josef Brozek and Ludwig J. Pongratz , 255-78. Toronto: C.J. Hogrefe, Inc., 1980.
Google Scholar
__. ed. An Education in Psychology: James McKeen Cattell's Journal and Letters from Germany and England, 1880-1888. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1981.
Google Scholar
—.' The Unpublished Autobiography of James McKeen Cattell.' American Psychologist 26 (July 1971): 626-35.
Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI
Wissler, Clark. 'The Correlation of Mental and Physical Tests.' Psychological Review Monograph Supplements 3 (Whole No. 16) (June 1901): 1-62.
Google Scholar

James McKeen Cattell (May 25, 1860 – January 20, 1944), Americanpsychologist, was the first professor of psychology in the United States, teaching at the University of Pennsylvania, and long-time editor and publisher of scientific journals and publications, most notably the journal Science. He also served on the board of trustees for Science Service, now known as Society for Science & the Public (or SSP), from 1921–1944.

At the beginning of his career, many scientists regarded psychology at best a minor field of study, or at worst a pseudoscience such as phrenology. Perhaps more than any of his contemporaries, Cattell helped establish psychology as a legitimate science, worthy of study at the highest levels of the academy. At the time of his death, The New York Times hailed him as 'the dean of American science.' Yet Cattell may be best remembered for his uncompromising opposition to American involvement in World War I.[1] His public opposition to the draft led to his dismissal from his position at Columbia University, a move that later led many American universities to establish tenure as a means of protecting unpopular beliefs.[1]

James McKeen Cattell
BornMay 25, 1860
DiedJanuary 20, 1944 (aged 83)
NationalityAmerican
Alma materUniversity of Leipzig (PhD)
Lafayette College (MA, BA)
Scientific career
Fieldspsychologist, psychometrics
InstitutionsUniversity of Cambridge
University of Pennsylvania
Columbia University
Doctoral advisorWilhelm Wundt
Doctoral studentsWalter Dearborn

Early life

Born in Easton, Pennsylvania in 1860, Cattell grew up the eldest child of a wealthy and prominent family. His father, William Cassady Cattell, a Presbyterian minister, became president of Lafayette College in Easton shortly after James' birth. William Cattell could easily provide for his children, as he had married Elizabeth 'Lizzie' McKeen in 1859; together they shared Lizzie's substantial inheritance. To this picture of the family's success one could add political power as well, as James' uncle Alexander Gilmore Cattell represented New Jersey in the United States Senate.

Cattell entered Lafayette College in 1876 at the age of sixteen, and graduated in four years with the highest honors. In 1883 the faculty at Lafayette awarded him an M.A., again with highest honors. Despite his later renown as a scientist, he spent most of his time devouring English literature, although he showed a remarkable gift for mathematics as well. Cattell said Francis Andrew March, a philologist, was a great influence during his time at Lafayette.[2]

Cattell did not find his calling until after he arrived in Germany for graduate studies, where he met Wilhelm Wundt at the University of Leipzig. He also studied under Hermann Lotze at the University of Göttingen, and an essay on Lotze won him a fellowship at Johns Hopkins University, where he left Germany to study in October 1882.[2] The fellowship was not renewed,[2] and he returned to Leipzig the next year as Wundt's assistant.

The partnership between Wundt and Cattell proved highly productive, as the two helped to establish the formal study of intelligence. Under Wundt, Cattell became the first American to publish a dissertation in the field of psychology. The title of his German dissertation was Psychometrische Untersuchungen (Psychometric Investigation). The dissertation was accepted by the University of Leipzig in 1886. More controversially, Cattell tried to explore the interiors of his own mind through the consumption of the then-legal drug hashish. Under the influence of this drug, Cattell once compared the whistling of a schoolboy to a symphony orchestra. While recreational drug use was not uncommon among early psychologists, including Freud, Cattell's experimentation with hashish reflected a willingness to go against conventional opinion and morality.

The main street in the College Hill Neighborhood of Easton, Pennsylvania, home to Lafayette College, is named after Cattell.

Academic career

After completing his Ph.D. with Wundt in Germany in 1886, Cattell took up a lecturing post at the University of Cambridge in England, and became a 'Fellow Commoner' of St John's College, Cambridge.[3] He made occasional visits to America where he gave lectures at Bryn Mawr and the University of Pennsylvania. In 1889 he returned to the United States to take up the post of Professor of Psychology in Pennsylvania, and in 1891 moved to Columbia University where he became Department Head of Psychology, Anthropology, and Philosophy; He became President of the American Psychological Association in 1895.

From the beginning of his career, Cattell worked hard to establish psychology as a field as worthy of study as any of the 'hard' physical sciences, such as chemistry or physics. Indeed, he believed that further investigation would reveal that the intellect itself could be parsed into standard units of measurements. He also brought the methods of Wilhelm Wundt and Francis Galton back to the United States, establishing the mental testing efforts in the U.S.

In 1917, Cattell and English professor Henry Wadsworth Longfellow Dana (grandson of Henry Wadsworth Longfellow and Richard Henry Dana Jr.) were fired from Columbia University for opposing the United States’ conscription policy during World War I.[4] Years later he sued the university and won an annuity. In 1921, he used the money that he had gained from the settlement in order to start The Psychological Corporation to foster his interest in the field of applied psychology. Because he was never able to really explain how psychologists can apply their work, the organization failed until taken over by other psychologists who had experience in applied psychology. Towards the end of his life, Cattell still edited and published his journals. To help himself in the process, he created the Science Press Printing Company in order to produce his journals. He continued his work on the journals until his death in 1944 in Lancaster, Pennsylvania.

Eugenicist beliefs

Like many eminent scientists and scholars of the time, Cattell's thought was influenced by belief in eugenics, defined as the 'applied science or the bio-social movement which advocates the use of practices aimed at improving the genetic composition of a population, usually referring to human populations.'[5] Cattell's belief in eugenics was heavily influenced by the research of Charles Darwin, whose theory of evolution motivated Cattell’s emphasis on studying “the psychology of individual differences”.[6]

In connection with his eugencist beliefs, Cattell's own research found that men of science were likely to have fathers who were clergymen or professors. Incidentally, Cattell's father was both.[7]

Cattell believed that he had “inherited ability', but he also credited the influence of his environment, saying 'it was my fortune to find a birthplace in the sun. A germplasm fairly well compounded [good genes] met circumstances to which it was unusually fit to react”.[7] Cattell’s belief in eugenics even motivated him to offer his own children monetary gifts of $1,000 if they married the offspring of a university professor or academic professional.[8]

Mental tests

James

Cattell’s research on individual differences played a significant role in introducing and emphasizing the experimental technique and importance of methodology in experimentation in America.[9] Regarding the beginnings of his mental tests, in Leipzig, Cattell independently began to measure “simple mental processes”[8] Between 1883 and 1886, Cattell published nine articles discussing human reaction time rates and individual differences.[8] As professor at the University of Pennsylvania, Cattell administered a battery of ten tests to student volunteers, and for the first time introduced the term “mental tests” as a general term for his set of tests which included measures of sensation, using weights to determine just-noticeable differences, reaction time, human memory span, and rate of movement.[8] When Cattell moved to Columbia University, the battery of tests became compulsory for all freshmen. Cattell believed that his mental tests were measuring intelligence; however, in 1901 Clark Wissler, a student of Cattell, demonstrated that there was no statistical relationship between scores on Cattell's tests and academic performance. The tests were finally rendered irrelevant with the development of Alfred Binet’s intelligence measurements.[8][10]

Journals

Cattell was well known for his involvement in creating and editing scientific journals. He was so involved in owning and publishing journals, that his research productivity declined. He founded the journal Psychological Review in 1894 along with James Mark Baldwin. He also acquired the journal Science and, within five years, made it the official publication of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1895-1900. In 1900, he purchased Popular Science Monthly from D. Appleton & Company. In 1915, the title was purchased from him and became Popular Science. He, in turn, founded and edited The Scientific Monthly, which went to the subscribers of the old Popular Science Monthly as a substitute.[9][11]

James Mckeen Cattell Pdf

Cattell was the editor of Science for nearly 50 years. During that time, he did much to promote psychology as a science by seeing to it that empirical studies in psychology were prominently featured in the journal.[8] Regarding his impact on the development of psychology as a science, Ludy T. Benjamin wrote of Cattell's editorship “there is no denying that it significantly enhanced psychology’s visibility and status among the older sciences.”[6]:56

Skepticism

Cattell was skeptical of paranormal claims and spiritualism. He had dismissed the medium Leonora Piper as a fraud. He was involved in a debate over Piper with the psychologist William James in the Science journal.[12][13][14] He took issue with James's support for psychical research.[15] In a letter to James he wrote that the 'Society for Psychical Research is doing much to injure psychology'.[16]

Family

He married Josephine Owen, the daughter of an English merchant, in 1888. Their seven children got their pre-college educations at home with their parents as instructors. The whole family shared in Cattell's editorial work.[2] One daughter, Psyche Cattell (b. 1893) followed in her father's footsteps, established a small child psychology practice in Lancaster, Pennsylvania and developed tests to assess the intelligence of infants.[17]

References

  1. ^ abJonathan Baron (2006). 'History: Cattell at Penn'. Retrieved 2014-09-24. Baron cites C. S. Gruber (1972), 'Academic freedom at Columbia University: The case of James McKeen Cattell', AAUP Bulletin, Autumn, pp. 297-305, with respect to Cattell's views on the war and place in the debate on academic freedom.
  2. ^ abcdDorothy Ross (1973). 'Cattell, James McKeen'. Dictionary of American Biography. Supplement Three (1941-1945). New York: Charles Scribner's Sons.
  3. ^'Cattell, James McKeen (CTL886JM)'. A Cambridge Alumni Database. University of Cambridge.
  4. ^Current Opinion, November 1917, p.294
  5. ^'Eugenics', Unified Medical Language System (Psychological Index Terms), National Library of Medicine, 26 Sep. 2010.
  6. ^ abBenjamin, L.T., Jr. (1993). A history of psychology in letters. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown. ISBN 978-0697129802. OCLC26931839.
  7. ^ abSokal, M. M. (1980). 'Science and James McKeen Cattell, 1894 to 1945'. Science. 209 (4452): 43–52. doi:10.1126/science.7025202. PMID7025202.
  8. ^ abcdefThorne, B. M., Henley, T. (2001). Connections in the history and systems of psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin
  9. ^ abPillsbury, W.B. (1947). Biographical Memoir of James McKeen Cattell 1860-1944. National Academy of the Sciences. http://www.nasonline.org/publications/biographical-memoirs/memoir-pdfs/cattell-james-m.pdf
  10. ^Applied History of Psychology/Models of Testing. Wikibooks. http://en.wikibooks.org/wiki/Applied_History_of_Psychology/Models_of_Assessment
  11. ^Cattell, James McKeen (September 1915). 'The Scientific Monthly and the Popular Science Monthly'. Popular Science Monthly. 87 (3): 307–310.
  12. ^Cattell, J. M. (1898). Mrs. Piper, the medium. Science 7: 534-535.
  13. ^James, W. (1898). Mrs. Piper, 'the medium'.Science 7: 640-641.
  14. ^Cattell, J. M. (1898). Mrs. Piper, 'the medium'. Science 7: 641-642.
  15. ^Kimble, Gregory A; Wertheimer, Michael; White, Charlotte. (2013). Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology. Psychology Press. p. 23. ISBN 0-8058-0620-2
  16. ^Goodwin, C. James. (2015). A History of Modern Psychology. Wiley. p. 154. ISBN 978-1-118-83375-9
  17. ^Cattell, Psyche (1940). The Measurement of Intelligence of Infants and Young Children (1st ed.). New York, New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Further reading

  • Cattell, James McKeen (1886). 'Psychometrische Untersuchungen'.
  • Cattell, James McKeen (1921). American Men of Science: A Biographical Directory (3rd ed.). Garrison, New York: The Science Press.
  • Godin (2007). 'From eugenics to scientometrics: Galton, Cattell, and men of science'. Social Studies of Science. 37 (5): 691–728. doi:10.1177/0306312706075338. PMID18348397.
  • Whipple (2004). 'Eminence revisited'. History of Psychology. 7 (3): 265–96. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.7.3.265. PMID15382378.
  • Benschop; Draaisma, D (2000). 'In pursuit of precision: the calibration of minds and machines in late nineteenth-century psychology'. Annals of Science. 57 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1080/000337900296281. PMID11624166.
  • Cattell (1992). 'Retrospect: psychology as a profession. 1937'. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 60 (1): 7–8, discussion 9–15. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.60.1.7. PMID1556288.
  • Sokal (1980). 'Science and James McKeen Cattell, 1894 to 1945'. Science. 209 (4452): 43–52. doi:10.1126/science.7025202. PMID7025202.
  • Sokal (1971). 'The unpublished autobiography of James McKeen Cattell'. The American Psychologist. 26 (7): 626–35. doi:10.1037/h0032048. PMID4934276.

External links

  • Short biography, bibliography, and links on digitized sources in the Virtual Laboratory of the Max Planck Institute for the History of Science
  • Works by James McKeen Cattell at Project Gutenberg
  • Works by or about James McKeen Cattell at Internet Archive
  • James McKeen Cattell at Find a Grave
Alice F. Healy

James Mckeen Cattell Psychology Paper

Alice Fenvessy Healy is a psychologist and College Professor of Distinction Emeritus at the University of Colorado Boulder where she founded and directed the Center for Research on Training. She is known for her research in the field of cognitive psychology, spanning diverse topics including short-term memory, long-term memory, psycholinguistics, reading, decision-making, and cognitive training.Healy and her colleague Lyle E. Bourne, Jr. are authors of the book Train Your Mind for Peak Performance: A Science-Based Approach for Achieving Your Goals and the textbook Cognitive Processes with Roger Dominowski and Elizabeth Loftus. Healy and Bourne co-edited several volumes together including Training Cognition: Optimizing Efficiency, Durability, and Generalizability, Foreign Language Learning: Psycholinguistic Studies on Training and Retention, and Learning and Memory of Knowledge and Skills: Durability and Specificity. Healy co-edited, with Robert Proctor, the Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4 Experimental Psychology. With Stephen Kosslyn and Richard Shiffrin, she co-edited a two-volume Festschrift in honor of her doctoral advisor, William Kaye Estes.Healy is a fellow of the American Psychological Association (Division 1 and Division 3), the Association for Psychological Science, the American Association for the Advancement of Science and the Society of Experimental Psychologists. She has served as President of Division 3 (Experimental Psychology) of the American Psychological Association, as Chair of the Psychology Section of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, and as Chair of the Society of Experimental Psychologists. She was recipient of the James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award and the Women in Cognitive Science Mentorship Award. Healy was honored by the Federation of Associations of the Behavioral and Brain Sciences as a scientist who 'made important and lasting contributions to the sciences of mind, brain, and behavior' in 2014. That year, her career was celebrated with a Festschrift, with participants contributing to a Special Issue of the American Journal of Psychology.

Cognitive test

Cognitive tests are assessments of the cognitive capabilities of humans and other animals. Tests administered to humans include various forms of IQ tests; those administered to animals include the mirror test (a test of visual self-awareness) and the T maze test (which tests learning ability). Such study is important to research concerning the philosophy of mind and psychology, as well as determination of human and animal intelligence.

Modern cognitive tests originated through the work of James McKeen Cattell who coined the term 'mental tests'. They followed Francis Galton's development of physical and physiological tests. For example, Galton measured strength of grip and height and weight. He established an 'Anthropometric Laboratory' in the 1880s where patrons paid to have physical and physiological attributes measured. Galton's measurements had an enormous influence on psychology. Cattell continued the measurement approach with simple measurements of perception. Cattell's tests were eventually abandoned in favor of the battery test approach developed by Alfred Binet.

Diane F. Halpern

Diane F. Halpern is an American psychologist and former president of the American Psychological Association (APA). She is Dean of Social Science at the Minerva Schools at KGI (Keck Graduate Institute) and also the McElwee Family Professor of Psychology at Claremont McKenna College. She is also past-president of the Western Psychological Association, The Society for the Teaching of Psychology, and the Division of General Psychology.

Halpern served on boards and panels at the APA including the Board of Educational Affairs, the Council of Representatives, the Committee on International Relations in Psychology, the Committee on Learning Outcomes, and Teachers of Psychology in Secondary Schools. She chaired the Panel on Public Policy and APA's National Conference on Undergraduate Education that resulted in the publication Undergraduate Education in Psychology: A Blueprint for the Future of the Discipline.

Along with Keith Millis (Northern Illinois University) and Arthur C. Graesser (University of Memphis), Halpern developed Operation ARA, a computerized game that teaches scientific reasoning. She also developed the Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment (Schuhfried Publishers) that uses multiple response formats which allow test-takers to demonstrate their ability to think about everyday topics using both constructed response and recognition formats.

Halpern received her B.A. from University of Pennsylvania and an M.A. from Temple University. She then received an M.A., from University of Cincinnati, followed by a Ph.D. from that institution in 1979. After teaching for many years at the California State University, San Bernardino, she is currently Professor of Psychology at Claremont McKenna College.

In 1995, Halpern was part of an 11-member APA task force led by Ulric Neisser which published Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns, a report written in response to The Bell Curve. She has also written on cognitive differences between men and women. She suggests a biopsychosocial model offers superior insight into cognitive sex differences than a simple nature-vs-nurture dichotomy. Another topic of her research is risks associated with left-handedness. Halpern has also written in opposition of single-sex schooling, noting that it 'lacks scientific support and may exaggerate sexism and gender stereotyping.'Halpern has won awards for her teaching and research, including:

2013 James McKeen Cattell Award from the Association for Psychological Science (APS) (highest award given by APS)

2013 American Psychological Foundation Arthur W. Staats Award

2009 George A. Miller Award for the outstanding journal article in psychology

2004 Honorary Doctorate of Humane Letters (Ph.D.) Mount St. Mary’s College (Los Angeles)

2002 Outstanding Professor Award from the Western Psychological Association

1999 American Psychological Foundation (APF) Charles L. Brewer Distinguished Teaching Award

1996 Distinguished Career Award for Contributions to Education given by the American Psychological Association

1996 California State University's State-Wide Outstanding Professor Award

1996 Outstanding Alumna Award from the University of Cincinnati

1996 Silver Medal Award from the Council for the Advancement and Support of Education

Wang Family Excellence Award

G. Stanley Hall Lecture Award from the American Psychological Association.

Dominic W. Massaro

Dominic W. Massaro is Professor of Psychology and Computer Engineering at the University of California, Santa Cruz. He is best known for his fuzzy logical model of perception, and more recently, for his development of the computer animated talking head Baldi. Massaro is director of the Perceptual Science Laboratory, past president of the Society for Computers in Psychology, book review editor for the American Journal of Psychology, founding Chair of UCSC's Digital Arts and New Media program, and was founding co-editor of the interdisciplinary journal Interpreting. He has been a Guggenheim Fellow, a University of Wisconsin Romnes Fellow, a James McKeen Cattell Fellow, an NIMH Fellow, and in 2006 was recognized as a Tech Museum Award Laureate.Massaro received his B.A. in Psychology from the University of California, Los Angeles in 1965, and completed his Ph.D. in Mathematical Psychology at the University of Massachusetts Amherst in 1968. After his postdoctoral work at the University of California, San Diego, he was Professor of Psychology at the University of Wisconsin, Madison from 1970 to 1979, before moving to UCSC where he has remained since.Massaro's research focuses on applying an information processing approach to the study of language, perception, memory, cognition, and decision making. In collaboration with Gregg Oden, he developed the fuzzy logical model of perception, which stresses the integration of multiple sources of information when modeling perception. This model has been shown to be mathematically equivalent to Bayes' theorem. Massaro's approach conflicts with the motor theory of speech perception and Massaro has been a critic.Stemming from this early work, Massaro established a research program demonstrating the importance of information from the face in speech perception. As part of this program, Massaro, along with researcher Michael Cohen, developed the computer animated talking head known as Baldi. The Baldi technology is special in its extraordinary accuracy, and has been expanded to speak in numerous languages. In recent years, Massaro has become more involved with applied research, using his talking head technology to benefit language learners, including those facing learning challenges such as deafness and autism. For this work, he was named a 2006 Tech Microsoft Education Award Laureate by the Tech Museum of Innovation.One focus of his current research is on the development and theoretical and applied use of a completely synthetic and animated head (iBaldi) for speech synthesis, language tutoring, and edutainment. Massaro founded Psyentific Mind, Inc with the goal of using Behavioral Science and Technology to extend the range of the human mind. The company currently has 8 iPhone/iPad apps in the Apple app store. These include apps for enhancing literacy, phonics, tile matching games for learning vocabulary and phonics, automated speech recognition in face to face communication, and learning to tell time with Kid Klok, an easy-to-read analog clock for children.

Massaro is husband to ceramicist Karen Thuesen Massaro and father to Pia T. Massaro and Andrew T. Massaro

Ellen Frank (scientist)

Ellen Frank (born 1944) is a psychologist and Distinguished Professor Emeritus of Psychiatry and Distinguished Professor of Psychology at the University of Pittsburgh. She is known in the field of Psychotherapy as one of the developers of Interpersonal and Social Rhythm Therapy, which aims to treat bipolar disorder by correcting disruptions in the circadian rhythm while promoting increased regularity of daily social routines. Frank is the co-founder and Chief Scientific Officer of HealthRhythms,[1] a company that uses mobile technology to monitor the health and mental health of clients, facilitate the detection of changes in their status, and better manage mental health conditions.Frank received the 2008 Award for Research in Mood Disorders from the American College of Psychiatrists, and the 2011 Rhoda and Bernard Sarnat International Prize in Mental Health from the National Academy of Medicine for her research on mood disorders and their treatment. She also received the 2015 James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award from the Association for Psychological Science.

Harry Levi Hollingworth

Harry Levi Hollingworth (May 26, 1880 – September 17, 1956) was one of the first psychologists to bring psychology into the advertising world, as well as a pioneer in applied psychology.

Jacques Cattell

Jacques (Jack) Cattell (2 June 1904 in Garrison, New York – 19 December 1961 in Scottsdale, Arizona) was an American publisher and founder of a company bearing his name, 'Jacques Cattell Press, Inc.,' based in Lancaster, Pennsylvania.

James Mark BaldwinJames mckeen cattell forensic psycho…

James Mark Baldwin (January 12, 1861, Columbia, South Carolina – November 8, 1934, Paris) was an American philosopher and psychologist who was educated at Princeton under the supervision of Scottish philosopher James McCosh and who was one of the founders of the Department of Psychology at the university. He made important contributions to early psychology, psychiatry, and to the theory of evolution.

James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award

The James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award is an award of the Association for Psychological Science given since 1992. The award is named after James McKeen Cattell and 'honors individuals for their lifetime of significant intellectual contributions to the basic science of psychology.' As part of APS’s 25th Anniversary, the APS Board of Directors recognized a larger class of James McKeen Cattell Fellows in 2013, identifying them as individuals who have had a profound impact on the field of psychological science over the previous quarter century.”

Janet Shibley Hyde

Janet Shibley Hyde is the Helen Thompson Woolley Professor of Psychology and Gender & Women's Studies at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. She is known for her research on human sexuality, sex differences, gender development, gender and science, and feminist theory, and is considered one of the leading academics in the field of gender studies.

Louis Matzel

Louis D. Matzel is a professor of psychology at Rutgers University. His research is in the fields of behavioral neuroscience and differential psychology, with a focus on individual differences in intelligence. He is also noted for his criticisms of the concept of long-term potentiation. He was a recipient of the James McKeen Cattell Fund Fellowship from the Association for Psychological Science in 1999–2000, and he has been a fellow of the Eastern Psychological Association since 2009.

Michael Lamb (psychologist)

Michael E. Lamb is a professor and former Head of the then Department of Social and Developmental Psychology at the University of Cambridge. In 2003 Lamb was the recipient of the 2003–2004 James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award from the Association for Psychological Science.

Michael Sokal

Michael Sokal is a retired professor at Worcester Polytechnic Institute in the history of science. He received his PhD in history of science and technology from Case Western Reserve University in 1972. His research focuses on James McKeen Cattell, a prominent psychologist and scientific impresario in the late 19th and early 20th centuries. He was the 2004-2005 president of the History of Science Society.

Neal Schmitt

Neal William Schmitt (born 1944) is an American psychologist specializing in personnel psychology and industrial and organizational psychology. He is a University Distinguished Professor in the Department of Psychology at Michigan State University, where he previously served as interim dean of the College of Social Science. He is a former editor-in-chief of the Journal of Applied Psychology.In 2014, Schmitt received the James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award from the Association for Psychological Science. In 2015, he and Robert Sellers jointly received the Award for Distinguished Service to Psychological Science from the American Psychological Association.

Psychological Review

Psychological Review is a scientific journal that publishes articles on psychological theory. It was founded by Princeton University psychologist James Mark Baldwin and Columbia University psychologist James McKeen Cattell in 1894 as a publication vehicle for psychologists not connected with the Clark laboratory of G. Stanley Hall (who often published in Hall's American Journal of Psychology). Psychological Review soon became the most prominent and influential psychology journal in North America, publishing important articles by William James, John Dewey, James Rowland Angell, and many others.

Sandra Scarr

Sandra Wood Scarr (born August 8, 1936) is an American psychologist and writer. She was the first female full professor in psychology in the history of Yale University. She established core resources for the study of development, including the Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study and the Minnesota Adolescent Adoption Study. She served as president of multiple societies including the Association for Psychological Science and was honoured with multiple awards including the James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award. She was also active in the development of commercial childcare. Her work with twins in the 1960s revealed strong genetic influences on intellectual development. One of her key findings was that this differed with race and socioeconomic status (SES), with poor and non-white children showing less genetic influence on their IQ and more environmental influence. She demonstrated a successful intervention in premature infants, showing that stimulation improved their health and developmental outcomes.At Minnesota, she and Richard A. Weinberg found that black and interracial children adopted early into white homes initially had outcomes more similar to the white average, suggesting a role of family environment early in life. By their teens, adoptees with two black birth parents achieved lower scores than did adoptees with one or no black birth parents, suggesting a genetic component to race differences in IQ.Along with the Scarr-Rowe effect of socio-economic status on the heritability of intelligence, another key intellectual landmark established by Scarr was that 'Rather than the home environment having a cumulative impact across development, its influence wanes from early childhood to adolescence.' She sought also to advance scientific psychology, and in 1991 co-founded Current Directions In Psychological Science. She retired to, and continues to live in Hawaii.

The Scientific Monthly

The Scientific Monthly was a science magazine published from 1915 to 1957. Psychologist James McKeen Cattell, the former publisher and editor of The Popular Science Monthly, was the original founder and editor. In 1958, The Scientific Monthly was absorbed by Science.

Ward Edwards

Ward Edwards (1927–2005) was an American psychologist, prominent for work on decision theory and on the formulation and revision of beliefs.

Presidents of the American Psychological Association
1892–1900
  • G. Stanley Hall (1892)
  • George Trumbull Ladd (1893)
  • William James (1894)
  • James McKeen Cattell (1895)
  • George Stuart Fullerton (1896)
  • James Mark Baldwin (1897)
  • Hugo Münsterberg (1898)
  • John Dewey (1899)
  • Joseph Jastrow (1900)
1901–1925
  • Josiah Royce (1901)
  • Edmund Sanford (1902)
  • William Lowe Bryan (1903)
  • William James (1904)
  • Mary Whiton Calkins (1905)
  • James Rowland Angell (1906)
  • Henry Rutgers Marshall (1907)
  • George M. Stratton (1908)
  • Charles Hubbard Judd (1909)
  • Walter Bowers Pillsbury (1910)
  • Carl Seashore (1911)
  • Edward Thorndike (1912)
  • Howard C. Warren (1913)
  • Robert S. Woodworth (1914)
  • John B. Watson (1915)
  • Raymond Dodge (1916)
  • Robert Yerkes (1917)
  • John Wallace Baird (1918)
  • Walter Dill Scott (1919)
  • Shepherd Ivory Franz (1920)
  • Margaret Floy Washburn (1921)
  • Knight Dunlap (1922)
  • Lewis Terman (1923)
  • G. Stanley Hall (1924)
  • I. Madison Bentley (1925)
1926–1950
  • Harvey A. Carr (1926)
  • Harry Levi Hollingworth (1927)
  • Edwin Boring (1928)
  • Karl Lashley (1929)
  • Herbert Langfeld (1930)
  • Walter Samuel Hunter (1931)
  • Walter Richard Miles (1932)
  • Louis Leon Thurstone (1933)
  • Joseph Peterson (1934)
  • Albert Poffenberger (1935)
  • Clark L. Hull (1936)
  • Edward C. Tolman (1937)
  • John Dashiell (1938)
  • Gordon Allport (1939)
  • Leonard Carmichael (1940)
  • Herbert Woodrow (1941)
  • Calvin Perry Stone (1942)
  • John Edward Anderson (1943)
  • Gardner Murphy (1944)
  • Edwin Ray Guthrie (1945)
  • Henry Garrett (1946)
  • Carl Rogers (1947)
  • Donald Marquis (1948)
  • Ernest Hilgard (1949)
  • J. P. Guilford (1950)
1951–1975
  • Robert Richardson Sears (1951)
  • J. McVicker Hunt (1952)
  • Laurance F. Shaffer (1953)
  • Orval Hobart Mowrer (1954)
  • E. Lowell Kelly (1955)
  • Theodore Newcomb (1956)
  • Lee Cronbach (1957)
  • Harry Harlow (1958)
  • Wolfgang Köhler (1959)
  • Donald O. Hebb (1960)
  • Neal E. Miller (1961)
  • Paul E. Meehl (1962)
  • Charles E. Osgood (1963)
  • Quinn McNemar (1964)
  • Jerome Bruner (1965)
  • Nicholas Hobbs (1966)
  • Gardner Lindzey (1967)
  • Abraham Maslow (1968)
  • George Armitage Miller (1969)
  • George Albee (1970)
  • Kenneth B. Clark (1971)
  • Anne Anastasi (1972)
  • Leona E. Tyler (1973)
  • Albert Bandura (1974)
  • Donald T. Campbell (1975)
1976–2000
  • Wilbert J. McKeachie (1976)
  • Theodore H. Blau (1977)
  • M. Brewster Smith (1978)
  • Nicholas Cummings (1979)
  • Florence Denmark (1980)
  • John J. Conger (1981)
  • William Bevan (1982)
  • Max Siegel (1983)
  • Janet Taylor Spence (1984)
  • Robert Perloff (1985)
  • Logan Wright (1986)
  • Bonnie Strickland (1987)
  • Raymond D. Fowler (1988)
  • Joseph Matarazzo (1989)
  • Stanley Graham (1990)
  • Charles Spielberger (1991)
  • Jack Wiggins Jr. (1992)
  • Frank Farley (1993)
  • Ronald E. Fox (1994)
  • Robert J. Resnick (1995)
  • Dorothy Cantor (1996)
  • Norman Abeles (1997)
  • Martin Seligman (1998)
  • Richard Suinn (1999)
  • Patrick H. DeLeon (2000)
2001–Present
  • Norine G. Johnson (2001)
  • Philip Zimbardo (2002)
  • Robert Sternberg (2003)
  • Diane F. Halpern (2004)
  • Ronald F. Levant (2005)
  • Gerald Koocher (2006)
  • Sharon Brehm (2007)
  • Alan E. Kazdin (2008)
  • James H. Bray (2009)
  • Carol D. Goodheart (2010)
  • Melba J. T. Vasquez (2011)
  • Suzanne Bennett Johnson (2012)
  • Donald N. Bersoff (2013)
  • Nadine Kaslow (2014)
  • Barry S. Anton (2015)
  • Susan H. McDaniel (2016)
  • Antonio Puente (2017)
  • Jessica Henderson Daniel (2018)
  • Rosie Phillips Davis (2019)

This page is based on a Wikipedia article written by authors (here).
Text is available under the CC BY-SA 3.0 license; additional terms may apply.
Images, videos and audio are available under their respective licenses.